The time spent by the pigs manipulating straw increased over 10% ranging up to 27% in all the pens receiving over 200g of straw per day compared to the range from 4-22% in pens receiving 20-100g of straw. Meanwhile, the time spent in redirected behavior decreased below 5% in all the pens receiving over 200g of straw per day. No significant differences were found when comparing pens provided with straw once or four times per day.
Keywords: Growing-finishing pigs; Straw; Redirected behavior; Manipulation
The provision of straw is a well-studied means of reducing redirected behavior towards pen mates as concluded in a review by Studnitz et al. [2]. The provision of straw appears to be more successful in reducing redirected behavior than supplying more space [2,5]. Straw also has the capacity to keep pigs interested even in the long-term [6-8]. Studies show that when more straw is provided, the more exploratory behavior is directed towards the straw and not to the pen mates [9]. Amounts of up to 2 kg straw per day show a proportional increase in exploratory behavior directed towards the material and decrease in redirected behavior [2,10].
The EU Commission Directive (2001/93/EC) states that: ''pigs must have permanent access to sufficient quantity of material to enable proper investigation and manipulation activities, such as straw, hay, wood, sawdust, mushroom compost, peat or a mixture of such, which does not compromise the health of the animals''.
In the Swedish animal welfare legislation (Jordbruksverket) [11] the provision of straw is mentioned in chapter 3, 7 § as:"... bedding product shall be given in such quantities so that they meet the pigs' needs for exploration and comfort....".
The EU directive is made to meet the exploratory needs of the pigs and reduce abnormal behaviors directed towards pen mates, thus improving welfare. However, when straw is provided it may clog the slats and drainage so that the removal of feces is hampered unless appropriate slat and slot widths and straw dimensions are used [12]. The management of straw increases the work for the animal keeper and involves extra costs.
Neither the Commission Directive nor the Swedish animal welfare regulation mentions the amount of straw that ought to be provided. Furthermore, the minimum amount of straw needed to satisfy the pigs motivation and the minimum amount of straw that reduce abnormal behavior directed towards pen mates needs further investigation. Therefore, the aim of this study was to identify the minimal amounts of straw needed to satisfy pig motivation for manipulation and reduce to a minimum the manipulating behavior of pigs directed toward pen mates.
The hypothesis is that the more straw that is provided, the more manipulative behavior towards straw will be performed and less redirected behaviors will be performed. Furthermore, provision of straw several times per day will result in more manipulative behavior directed towards straw and less behavior directed towards pen mates.
• 7 different amounts of straw (20, 40, 60, 80, 100, 200 or 300grams/ pig/ day)
• Straw rationed once daily (10 am) or divided in four equal batches times per day (10 am, 12pm, 2pm and 4pm).
The straw daily provisions were each weighed separately. Those groups that received straw four times per day were placed with a wall separating them from the groups which only received straw once daily, to minimize the effect of one group to the other.
Feeding took place at 8.30 am and at 2.30 pm. The behavior was observed in detail by one observer throughout the study using focal animals sampling and continuous recording for one hour between 9 and 10 am and between 3 and 4 pm. At the time of observation, the observer did not receive any information about which treatment each pen was subjected to. The observations were done after feeding with the assumption that the pigs would have a strong motivation to perform manipulative behavior after feeding [2].
Observations: The behaviors observed are presented in the ethogram in table 1. The observations began following the first focal animal and every time the animal changed its behavior a note was made in an Excel file. All four pigs in the pen served as focal animal during 15 minutes of each hour of observation.
When a starting time had been decided a focal animal was chosen at random. The order in which the other pigs in the pen should be observed was then randomly picked.
In the current study one "bout" is the time spent in one behavior without any interruptions of other behaviors from the ethogram except "walking" or "resting".
If a clear distinction of a behavior could not be determined then the behavior was noted as missing value.
Data processing: The total time spent in each behavior was calculated. The behavior "riding" was removed because no incidence of that behavior was recorded during the periods of observation. The behaviors "nibbling", "tail bite" and "ear bite" were combined into "redirected behavior" which is the term used in the following.
The statistical analysis was made using multiple linear regressions in SAS (SAS Inst. Inc., Cary, NC, USA), [15] and descriptive analyses were done using the broken stick model and charts with trend lines in Microsoft Office: Excel 2007©.
The broken stick model was considered because of its possibility to point at a value on the minimal amount of straw to growing-finishing pigs. This value represents the point where an increase in the provision of straw no longer affects the behavior further. The data was run through the program Mat Lab (Math works Inc.) [16] which created a chart with a trend line. Optimally, the trend line should increase/decrease in the beginning to then slope out at a breaking point (c-value). Mat Lab is programmed to find the best possible breaking point within the range of the data.
Behavior variables |
Definition |
Resting |
Lying, standing, sitting without performing other behaviour |
Walking |
Taking steps without performing other behaviour |
Eating |
Nose in feed trough |
Drinking |
Manipulating water nipple |
Manipulating straw |
Nibbling/ rooting/ biting on floor |
Manipulating environment |
Nibbling/ rooting/ biting on surface above ground level |
Fence contact |
Interaction/ sniffing/ biting/ pawing one or more pigs in adjacent pen |
Aggression |
Two or more pigs in the same pen are fighting by oral contact |
Riding |
A pig is mounting another pig |
Nibbling |
A pig nibbles another pig |
Belly massage |
A pig is massaging another pig's belly/ side |
with a visible vertical movement |
|
Tail biting |
A pig is biting/ touching/ sucking another pig's tail |
Ear biting |
A pig is biting/ touching/ sucking another pig´s ear |
Other |
Behaviour not included in ethogram |
Polynomial trend line: The polynomial trend line of the
Behaviour |
One time straw per day |
Four times straw per day |
Walking |
2.9 |
3.5 |
Resting |
56.7 |
56.8 |
Eating |
17.3 |
15.9 |
Drinking |
2.5 |
2.1 |
Manipulating straw |
13.6 |
13.1 |
Manipulating environment |
2.9 |
2.4 |
Fence contact neighbouring pigs |
0.9 |
0.7 |
Aggression |
0.1 |
0.1 |
Redirected behaviour |
2.4 |
3.2 |
Belly massage |
0.4 |
1.9 |
Other |
0.3 |
0.3 |
Statistical Model |
One time straw per day |
Four times straw per day |
Combined data (1and4 times straw per day) |
Manipulating straw (% of time) |
|
|
|
Polynomial trend line |
0.28 |
0.26 |
0.26 |
Logarithmic trend line |
0.22 |
0.12 |
0.17 |
Redirected behaviour (% of time) |
|
|
|
Polynomial trend line |
0.02 |
0.11 |
0.05 |
Logarithmic trend line |
0.01 |
0.11 |
0.04 |
Logarithmic trend line: The logarithmic trend line shows that the time spent manipulating straw is increased with increased provision of straw. A difference can be seen when observing the time spent in manipulative behavior in pens given straw 300g per day. For all those pens, manipulative behavior directed towards straw occurred more than 10% of the time. Data also seem to be less spread compared to the pens which received 20-100g of straw per day (figure 1(c)). When combining data from all the pens, the behavior manipulate straw increased with increasing amounts of straw.
Polynomial trend line: The polynomial trend line of redirected behavior seems to level out somewhere between 100- 200g of straw but the minimum is closer to 300g (figure 2(b)).
Logarithmic trend line: The logarithmic trend lines show that time spent in redirected behavior was reduced with increased provision of straw (figure 2(c)).
The time for observations during the day were decided to take place as close to feeding as possible but still giving the pigs enough time to finish eating before the onset of the observations. During the first minutes of each observation the pigs were usually occupied in eating behavior which suggests that the time for observations and feeding should be further apart.
Analysis of data: The r-square values of the charts from the current study are very low (Table 3) indicating that the trend lines can only explain up to 30% of the variation in the data. The r-square values of the polynomial trend lines appear to be somewhat bigger than for the logarithmic trend lines. This suggests that the polynomial trend line describe the data better than the logarithmic.
The broken stick model proved insufficient in producing a valid breaking point for the behavior "manipulating straw" inside the limits of this study. Several studies show that the provision of straw up to 2 kg per day [10] and above [6] results in a proportional increase in explorative behavior directed towards straw. This further suggests that the amount of straw needed for pigs to perform a maximum of manipulative behavior towards straw is beyond the 300g which is the maximum amount observed in the current study.
Even if no maximum was found in the current study, a clear distinction was observed when comparing the pens receiving 20-100g of straw and 200-300g of straw. The time spent in manipulative behavior directed towards straw was increased over 10% ranging up to 27% in all the pens receiving over 200g of straw per day compared to the range from 4-22% in pens receiving 20-100g of straw. The data are also less varied from pens provided with over 200g of straw which suggests that a larger proportion of the animals were affected more equally to the treatment. As opposed to increased feeding frequencies [19] no effects on manipulating or redirected behaviors were found in this study.
Redirected behavior: The hypothesis that the prevalence of redirected behavior should decrease with increasing amounts of straw is accepted. For the broken stick method, when the pens were combined regardless of the provisions, a breaking point at 200g of straw was found after which no further improvements were seen. A clear distinction can be observed when comparing these pigs with the ones provided with 20-100g of straw. The pigs from pens provided between 20-100g of straw spent 0-24% of the time in redirected behavior. The variation in these pens was also large and for each amount of straw, at least one pen reached above 5%. However, all the pigs from pens provided more than 100g of straw spent less than 5% of the time in redirected behavior and the data from these pens were more cohesive.
Camerlink and Turner [20] studied the occurrence of nosing behavior directed towards pen mates and concluded that it often consists of a gentle nose-to-nose contact and nosing of the head and body. The authors define this behavior as social nosing and it is performed "for social recognition, as an affiliative behavior, to gain olfactory signals, or to satisfy an intrinsic need to nose". This social nosing should not be associated with injurious behaviors. For the current study the term redirected behavior is a combination of the behaviors "nibbling", "tail bite" and "ear bite" and there is a possibility that the behavior "nibbling" include these social nosing episodes. If social nosing had been separated from harmful nosing in the current study the results may have been different.
- Stolba A, Wood-Gush DGM. The behavior of pigs in a semi-natural environment. Animal Production. 1989;48(2):419-425.
- Studnitz M, Jensen MB, Pedersen LJ. Why do pigs root and in what will they root?: A review on the exploratory behavior of pigs in relation to environmental enrichment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007;107(3):183- 197.
- EFSA. Scientific Opinion of the Panel on Animal Health and Welfare on a request from Commission. The risks associated with tail biting in pigs and possible means to reduce the need for tail docking considering the different housing and husbandry systems. The EFSA Journal. 2007;611:1-13.
- Fraser D, Phillips PA, Thompson BK, Tennessen T. Effect of straw on the behavior of growing pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1991;30(3- 4):307-318.
- Beattie VE, O'Connell NE. Relationship between rooting behaviour and foraging in growing pigs.Animal Welfare. 2002;11:295-303.
- Day JEL, Burfoot A, Docking CM, Whittaker X, Spoolder HAM, Edwards SA. The effects of prior experience of straw and the level of straw provision on the behavior of growing pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002;76:189-202.
- ZwickerB, Gygax L, Wechsler B, Weber R. Influence of the accessibility of straw in racks on exploratory behavior in finishing pigs. Livest. Sci. 2012;148(1-2):67-73.
- ZwickerB, Gygax L, Wechsler B, Weber R. Short- and long-term effects of eight enrichment materials on the behavior of finishing pigs fed ad libitum or restrictively. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013;144(1-2):31-38.
- Munsterhjelm C, Peltoniemi OAT, Heinonen M, Ha¨ ll O, Karhapa M, Valros A. Experience of moderate bedding affects behavior of growing pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009;118(1-2):42-53.
- Arey DS. Effect of straw on the behavior and performance of growing pigs in straw-flow pens. Farm Building Prog. 1993;112:24-25.
- Jordbruksverket. Föreskrifter om djurskydd för lantbrukets djur. 07/06/2015. http://www.jordbruksverket.se/amnesomraden/djur/ djurhalsopersonal/veterinaraforfattningshandboken/ldjurskydd.4.6 beab0f111fb74e78a78000821.html.
- Westin R, Holmgren N, Mattsson B, Algers B. Throughput capacity of large quantities of chopped straw in partly slatted farrowing pens for loose housed sows. Acta Agr. Scand. Sect. A Animal Science. 2013;63(1):18-27.
- Simonsson A. Fodermedel och näringsrekommendationer för gris. Institutionen för husdjurens utfodring och vård, Swed. Univ. Agric. Sci. Report. 266. 2006.
- MSH-Video, 07/06/2015. http://msh-video-client.software.informer. com/.
- http://msh-video-client.software.informer.com/
- Math works Inc. 07/06/2015. http://www.mathworks.se/products/ statistics/index.html.
- Schmolke SA, Li YZ, Gonyou HW.Effects of group size on social behavior following regrouping of growing–finishing pigs.Appl.Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004;88(1-2):27-38.
- Spicer HM, Aherne FX. The Effects of Group Size/Stocking Density on Weanling Pig Performance and Behavior. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.1987;19(1-2):89-98.
- Hessel EF, Wülbers-Mindermann M, Berg C, Van den Weghe HF, Algers B. Influence of increased feeding frequency on behavior and integument lesions in growing-finishing restricted-fed pigs. J Anim Sci. 2006;84(6):1526-1534.
- Camerlink I, Turner SP. The pig's nose and its role in dominance relationships and harmful behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013;145:84-91.